When should I trust my gut? Linking domain expertise to intuitive decision-making effectiveness

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.07.009Get rights and content

Abstract

Despite a growing body of scholarship on the concept of intuition, there is a scarcity of empirical research spotlighting the circumstances in which intuitive decision making is effective relative to analytical decision making. Seeking to address this deficiency, we conducted two laboratory studies assessing the link between domain expertise (low versus high) and intuitive decision-making effectiveness. These studies involved non-decomposable tasks across divergent expertise domains: basketball in Study 1 and designer handbag authentication in Study 2. Across both studies, and consistent with our overarching hypothesis, we found that the effectiveness of intuition relative to analysis is amplified at a high level of domain expertise. Taken together, our results demonstrate the importance of domain expertise in intuitive decision making and carry a number of theoretical and practical implications.

Highlights

► We relate domain expertise to intuitive decision-making effectiveness. ► We compare intuition and analysis on non-decomposable tasks. ► We reveal circumstances in which people should “trust their gut”.

Introduction

Emerging research has revived discussions surrounding the effectiveness of intuitive decision making. Traditionally, scholars have viewed intuitions as frequently biased and thus risky to base decisions upon (Dawes et al., 1989, Kahneman et al., 1982, Kahneman and Tversky, 2000). Therefore, analytical decision making, which is typically viewed as less susceptible to bias than intuition, has long been promoted by decision-making researchers (e.g., Janis and Mann, 1977, Keeney and Raiffa, 1976). Recently, however, interest in the benefits of intuition has been kindled by research and writings on the “intelligence” of nonconscious thought (George, 2009, Hofmann and Wilson, 2010), the potential accuracy of “thin-slice” judgments (Albrechtsen et al., 2009, Ambady, 2010, Ames et al., 2010), and the conditions under which “fast and frugal” heuristics foster effective decision making (Gigerenzer, 2007, Gigerenzer, 2008, Gigerenzer and Brighton, 2009).

Through the lines of research noted above, scholars have developed a richer understanding of the nature of intuition and have offered suggestions for when intuition should be enlisted in decision making (e.g., Betsch, 2008, Dane and Pratt, 2007, Epstein, 2010, Gore and Sadler-Smith, 2011, Hodgkinson et al., 2008, Hogarth, 2010, Sinclair, 2011). This growing interest in the topic of intuition is particularly evident in organizational scholarship. In today’s organizations there is increasing pressure across many ranks of workers to make decisions that are both rapid and effective (Perlow, Okhuysen, & Repenning, 2002). Doing so can prove challenging, however, because decision speed and decision effectiveness are often thought to be at odds (see Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2011 for a critique of this position). Intuition has been identified as a potential means by which to circumvent this apparent trade-off (Dane & Pratt, 2007). Indeed, some researchers argue that intuition represents a singular route toward making rapid decisions that are also effective (Hogarth, 2001, Sadler-Smith and Shefy, 2004).

These advances notwithstanding, empirical research on intuition remains limited. While it is argued that, in certain circumstances, there is merit to employing an intuitive decision-making approach (Hodgkinson and Sadler-Smith, 2003, Kahneman and Klein, 2009, Shapiro and Spence, 1997), there persists a lack of understanding and consensus about what these circumstances are. One possibility is that intuition tends to be effective on certain types of tasks (Hammond et al., 1987, Inbar et al., 2010, McMackin and Slovic, 2000). For example, research suggests that tasks which can be decomposed and approached sequentially tend to be conducive to analytical decision making, whereas tasks that are relatively non-decomposable are more suited to intuition (Hammond et al., 1987). However, the mere presence of a non-decomposable task may not ensure intuitive decision-making effectiveness. Research suggests that even for tasks well-suited to intuition, the effectiveness of intuitive decision making may relate to the level of expertise one has attained in the focal domain (Kahneman and Klein, 2009, Salas et al., 2010). Unfortunately, scant empirical research has directly investigated the connection between domain expertise and intuitive decision making effectiveness on these types of tasks. Seeking to address this deficiency, we examine here how domain expertise relates to intuitive (versus analytical) decision-making effectiveness on non-decomposable tasks across multiple expertise domains.

Section snippets

Intuitive versus analytical decision making

Following Dane and Pratt (2007, p. 40), we view intuitions as “affectively-charged judgments that arise through rapid, nonconscious, and holistic associations.” That is, the process of intuition, like nonconscious processing more generally, proceeds rapidly, holistically, and associatively (Betsch, 2008, Betsch and Glöckner, 2010, Sinclair, 2010).

Participants

Participants were recruited for this experiment from undergraduate business courses at a large Midwestern university. A total of 186 students participated in exchange for extra credit course points. Two participants were excluded from our analysis due to incomplete response forms, leaving a total of 184 participants (105 females). The number of participants who attended the experimental sessions ranged from 5 to 17.

Non-decomposable task and procedures

Upon arriving at the laboratory, participants were informed they would be

Intuition condition

In line with other approaches to inducing intuitive decision making (Jordan et al., 2007, Skarlicki and Rupp, 2010), we instructed participants in the intuition condition to base their decisions entirely on their intuition, or first impression, and to avoid thinking very hard about what the right answer is.

Analysis condition

Following previous approaches to inducing analytical decision making (McMackin and Slovic, 2000, Wilson and Schooler, 1991), we instructed participants in the analysis condition to base their decisions on a very careful analysis and to ignore any first impressions or “gut instincts” that might arise. Before the task began, we instructed these participants to develop a list of factors that would determine the difficulty of a basketball shot and told them to base their decisions on the factors

Participants

Participants were recruited for this experiment from undergraduate business courses at a large university in the Mid-Atlantic. A total of 242 students participated as part of a course requirement. Three participants were excluded from our analysis because they provided incomplete responses, leaving a total of 239 participants (119 females). The number of participants who attended the experimental sessions ranged from 8 to 14.

Non-decomposable task and procedures

Upon arriving at the laboratory, participants were told that they

General discussion

Through the studies reported here, we examined the link between domain expertise and intuitive decision making on non-decomposable tasks. Our results confirm our hypothesis that the effectiveness of intuition relative to analysis is amplified at a high level of domain expertise. As such, our research carries a number of implications worth considering. To begin, our results provide cause to reinterpret a rather fundamental research finding. In particular, it has been frequently demonstrated that

Limitations and future research

The studies reported here are subject to a critique common to many attempts to capture intuition empirically – how can one determine that a nonconscious process of judgment is actually taking place? Apart from advances in physiological and neurological approaches to intuition, which seek to identify changes in the brain or body associated with nonconscious processing (e.g., Bechara et al., 1997, Greene and Haidt, 2002, Lieberman, 2000, Lieberman, 2007), extant intuition research methodologies

Conclusion

The study of intuition has a long history in the social and behavioral sciences, yet there remains a lack of consensus about the circumstances in which intuition is an effective approach to making decisions. In the attempt to advance knowledge on this topic and answer the call for more empirical work on intuition (Dane & Pratt, 2009), we demonstrated that, on non-decomposable tasks, the effectiveness of intuition relative to analysis depends on one’s level of domain expertise. As such, the

Acknowledgments

We thank Matthew Cronin and Scott Sonenshein for their insightful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. We are also indebted to Sara Dane for her helpful suggestions and to Tina Borja for her copyediting assistance.

References (94)

  • J.W. Alba et al.

    Dimensions of consumer expertise

    Journal of Consumer Research

    (1987)
  • E.R. Alexander

    The design of alternatives in organizational contexts: A pilot study

    Administrative Science Quarterly

    (1979)
  • A.L. Alter et al.

    Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (2007)
  • N. Ambady

    The perils of pondering: Intuition and thin slice judgments

    Psychological Inquiry

    (2010)
  • D.R. Ames et al.

    Not so fast: The (not-quite-complete) dissociation between accuracy and confidence in thin-slice impressions

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2010)
  • M.H. Bazerman

    Judgment in managerial decision making

    (2006)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy

    Science

    (1997)
  • S.L. Beilock et al.

    Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills

    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

    (2004)
  • S.L. Beilock et al.

    When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied

    (2002)
  • T. Betsch et al.

    Intuition in judgment and decision making: Extensive thinking without effort

    Psychological Inquiry

    (2010)
  • C. Betsch et al.

    Measuring individual differences in intuitive and deliberate decision making styles: A comparison of different measures

  • C. Betsch et al.

    Individual strategy preferences and decisional fit

    Journal of Behavioral Decision Making

    (2008)
  • T. Betsch

    The nature of intuition and its neglect in research on judgment and decision making

  • P.F. Bundgaard

    Toward a cognitive semiotics of the visual artwork – Elements of a grammar of intuition

    Cognitive Semiotics

    (2009)
  • A.R. Damasio

    Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain

    (1994)
  • A.R. Damasio

    The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London (Biology)

    (1996)
  • E. Dane

    Reconsidering the trade-off between expertise and flexibility: A cognitive entrenchment perspective

    Academy of Management Review

    (2010)
  • E. Dane et al.

    Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making

    Academy of Management Review

    (2007)
  • E. Dane et al.

    Conceptualizing and measuring intuition: A review of recent trends

  • R.M. Dawes et al.

    Clinical versus actuarial judgment

    Science

    (1989)
  • H.L. Dreyfus et al.

    Expertise in real world contexts

    Organization Studies

    (2005)
  • B.D. Dunn et al.

    Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making

    Psychological Science

    (2010)
  • S. Epstein

    Demystifying intuition: What it is, what it does, and how it does it

    Psychological Inquiry

    (2010)
  • K.A. Ericsson et al.

    Expert performance. Its structures and acquisition

    American Psychologist

    (1994)
  • S. Frederick

    Cognitive reflection and decision making

    Journal of Economic Perspectives

    (2005)
  • J.M. George

    The illusion of will in organizational behavior research: Nonconscious processes and job design

    Journal of Management

    (2009)
  • G. Gigerenzer

    Gut feelings: The intelligence of the unconscious

    (2007)
  • G. Gigerenzer

    Why heuristics work

    Perspectives on Psychological Science

    (2008)
  • G. Gigerenzer et al.

    Homo heuristicus: Why biased minds make better inferences

    Topics in Cognitive Science

    (2009)
  • G. Gigerenzer et al.

    Heuristic decision making

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2011)
  • M. Gladwell

    Blink: The power of thinking without thinking

    (2005)
  • A. Glöckner et al.

    Foundations for tracing intuition: Challenges and methods

    (2010)
  • D.G. Goldstein et al.

    Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic

    Psychological Review

    (2002)
  • J. Gore et al.

    Unpacking intuition: A process and outcome framework

    Review of General Psychology

    (2011)
  • R. Gray

    Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking, and slumps

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied

    (2004)
  • J. Haidt

    The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment

    Psychological Review

    (2001)
  • K.R. Hammond

    Judgments under stress

    (2000)
  • Cited by (139)

    • The role of intuition in CEO acquisition decisions

      2023, Journal of Business Research
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text