Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Let them eat fruit! The effect of fruit and vegetable consumption on psychological well-being in young adults: A randomized controlled trial

  • Tamlin S. Conner ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Tamlin S. Conner, Kate L. Brookie

    tconner@psy.otago.ac.nz

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, Otago, New Zealand

  • Kate L. Brookie ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Tamlin S. Conner, Kate L. Brookie

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, Otago, New Zealand

  • Anitra C. Carr ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Centre for Free Radical Research, Department of Pathology, University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand

  • Louise A. Mainvil ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Human Nutrition, University of Otago, Dunedin, Otago, New Zealand

  • Margreet C. M. Vissers

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Centre for Free Radical Research, Department of Pathology, University of Otago, Christchurch, New Zealand

Abstract

This study tested the psychological benefits of a 14-day preregistered clinical intervention to increase fruit and vegetable (FV) consumption in 171 low-FV-consuming young adults (67% female, aged 18–25). Participants were randomly assigned into a diet-as-usual control condition, an ecological momentary intervention (EMI) condition involving text message reminders to increase their FV consumption plus a voucher to purchase FV, or a fruit and vegetable intervention (FVI) condition in which participants were given two additional daily servings of fresh FV to consume on top of their normal diet. Self-report outcome measures were depressive symptoms and anxiety measured pre- and post-intervention, and daily negative and positive mood, vitality, flourishing, and flourishing behaviors (curiosity, creativity, motivation) assessed nightly using a smartphone survey. Vitamin C and carotenoids were measured from blood samples pre- and post-intervention, and psychological expectancies about the benefits of FV were measured post-intervention to test as mediators of psychological change. Only participants in the FVI condition showed improvements to their psychological well-being with increases in vitality, flourishing, and motivation across the 14-days relative to the other groups. No changes were found for depressive symptoms, anxiety, or mood. Intervention benefits were not mediated by vitamin C, carotenoids, or psychological expectancies. We conclude that providing young adults with high-quality FV, rather than reminding them to eat more FV (with a voucher to purchase FV), resulted in significant short-term improvements to their psychological well-being. These results provide initial proof-of-concept that giving young adults fresh fruit and vegetables to eat can have psychological benefits even over a brief period of time.

Trial registration: Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry ACTRN12615000183583

Introduction

The physical health benefits of fruit and vegetables are well established. People who eat more fruit and vegetables (FV) have better cardiovascular health [1], reduced risk of some cancers [2], and greater longevity than people who eat fewer FV [3]. There is also growing evidence that people who eat more FV have better mental health. Higher consumption of FV is correlated with several psychological outcomes including a lower incidence of depression and anxiety [4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9], greater happiness [10, 11], higher life satisfaction [10, 12, 13, 14], and greater social-emotional well-being or “flourishing” [15]. This growing body of research is intriguing because it suggests that the foods people eat have a much broader impact beyond the notable physical health benefits. However, studies examining the psychological benefits of FV have been predominantly observational and epidemiological to date, with a relative lack of intervention research that would support a causal link.

Prospective research designs provide some evidence that increasing FV consumption may cause psychological benefits. At least five longitudinal studies found that dietary improvements including a diet higher in FV (i.e., a Mediterranean diet) predicted subsequent reductions in depression among adolescents, adults, and older adults [16, 17, 18, 19, 20]. However, these studies have not typically separated the effects of FV from other components of the Mediterranean diet such as olive oil or fish. More relevant are the results from Mujcic and Oswald [14] who analysed the prospective relationship between FV consumption and happiness and life satisfaction from the 2007 and 2009 waves of the Household, Income, and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) study, a nationally representative panel survey of over 12,000 people aged 15 years and older. They found that that increased FV consumption predicted greater “happiness gains” and increases in life satisfaction over the two years. Respondents with the largest increases in FV reported the greatest gains in well-being over time [14]. The opposite direction–happiness or life satisfaction predicting subsequent increases in FV consumption–was not found.

Only a few intervention studies have tested the psychological benefits of increased FV consumption directly. One study of 271 low-income participants in the UK showed improvements in the composite measure of mental health from the Rand Short Form Survey (SF-36 [21]) from baseline to 8-weeks follow up in participants who underwent brief nutritional and behavioral counselling to increase their FV consumption [22]. Another study of 35 healthy young men showed that eating two kiwifruits every day for six weeks resulted in modest improvements to mood as measured by the Profile of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire [6, 23]. Mood improvements corresponded with increased vitamin C levels, were only significant for the young men with poorer mood at the start of the study, and were mainly driven by reductions in fatigue, increases in “vigor” (which included reports of feeling cheerful, energetic, lively, and full of pep), and decreases in depressive symptoms (trend only) [6]. A third study of 100 university students showed that snacking on one piece of fruit (either an apple, large clementine, or banana) each day for 10 days resulted in reductions in fatigue and anxiety compared to daily snacking on chocolate wafers or potato chips [24]. Taken together, these studies support a causal link between FV and various measures of mental health and mood. Yet each study is limited in some way–either by a small sample size [6], exclusive focus on fruit [6, 24], or use of a composite mental health index that does not separate aspects of mental health such as depression and anxiety [22]. And, to our knowledge, no intervention study has tested whether FV improves aspects of well-being aside from the impact on ill-being. It is important to fill this gap given the growing observational and prospective evidence linking FV to indicators of well-being such as happiness and life satisfaction [10, 12, 13, 14], optimism [25], and flourishing [15].

Accordingly, in the present study, we tested the psychological benefits of a 14-day preregistered clinical intervention designed to increase fruit and vegetable (FV) consumption among low-FV-consuming young adults [26]. We defined low-consuming young adults as anyone aged 18 to 25 who reported eating fewer than 3 combined servings of fruit and vegetables per day. This cut-off reflects lower intake than the minimum national dietary guidelines of 5 combined servings of FV per day [27]. We implemented our intervention on a young adult population because young adults typically have the lowest FV consumption of all the age groups [28, 29, 30] and they are developing early autonomy over their health behaviors [31]. More details on the development of the intervention can be found in Brookie et al. [26]. To briefly summarize, we ran a three-arm randomized clinical trial (RCT) (n = 171) with three conditions: a diet-as-usual control condition in which participants were asked to maintain their regular food consumption for two weeks; an ecological momentary intervention condition (EMI), in which participants were sent twice daily text-messages that utilized a variety of behavioral change techniques to help them increase their fruit and vegetable consumption to at least 5 combined servings a day (plus they were given a voucher to purchase the FV); or a fruit and vegetable condition intervention (FVI), in which participants were given a bag of two weeks’ worth of fruit and vegetables (kiwifruit or oranges depending on the season, apples, and carrots) and were asked to consume at least two additional servings (1 fruit and 1 vegetable) on top of their regular daily FV consumption. Participants in both intervention conditions reported significantly higher daily FV consumption (3.7 servings / day) compared to control participants (2.8 servings / day), and compared to their own baseline (2.5 servings / day) when FV was reported nightly for two weeks using a smartphone-accessed survey [26]. Moreover, blood samples taken before and after the intervention showed that participants in the two experimental groups had small gains in vitamin C levels and plasma carotenoids, suggesting that the self-reported changes in FV consumption were legitimate. Vitamin C and carotenoids are the most consistently reliable biomarkers of FV consumption and have been used to reflect intake of nutrient-dense FV [32].

The current paper presents the psychological outcomes of that same two-week intervention reported in Brookie et al. [26]. The primary outcome measures were depressive symptoms and anxiety measured pre- and post-intervention, as well as negative and positive mood, vitality, and flourishing measured nightly through a smartphone survey. Our choice of methodology—smartphone tracking of daily psychological outcomes for two weeks—reflected our desire to minimise memory-based reporting and maximise sensitivity to detect possible group differences in mood and well-being changes over time [33]. Our choice of outcome measures reflected a desire to measure both the negative and positive aspects of mental health [34]. We predicted that participants in the two experimental conditions (EMI and FVI) would show significant improvements in the psychological outcomes relative to participants in the control condition, and that the benefits of FV consumption would be stronger for the measures of well-being (positive mood, vitality, flourishing). Although previous research has linked FV to lower levels ill-being–such as reduced depression [9, 35] and anxiety [9, 24]–there is more evidence linking FV to higher levels of well-being such as positive mood, happiness, and flourishing [6, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15]. We also included three unregistered “wild-card” variables in the daily smartphone survey–self-reported curiosity, creativity, and motivation–which we grouped under the umbrella term of flourishing behaviors. Higher FV consumption was previously shown to correlate with greater curiosity and creativity in young adults [15], but research has not yet established whether that relationship is causal. And, no research has tested whether FV might improve perceptions of motivation, a key correlate of vitality [36]. Lastly, we tested whether changes in the psychological outcomes would be mediated by two key biomarkers (vitamin C and carotenoids) and/or psychological expectancies. The mechanisms linking FV to well-being are unknown, but they may be due to key micronutrients like vitamin C and carotenoids, which act as cofactors for dopamine and other neurotransmitters involved in positive motivational states [37], or possibly due to positive psychological expectancies such as the perception that eating FV is virtuous or will make you feel better [38].

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

This is the same sample reported in Brookie et al. [26]. The experimental design and primary outcome measures were registered before recruitment with the Australia New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ANZCTRN12615000183583). Recruitment began in March 2015 and follow-up data was collected by the end of November 2015. Fig 1 shows the flow of participants throughout the trial and explanations for drop outs. Participants were 171 young adults (56 men; 115 women), 18 to 25 years old (M = 19.43, SD = 1.45) who identified as European (64%), Asian (18%), Māori or Pacific Islander (8%), or another or mixed ethnicity (11%). Participants were students at the University of Otago, New Zealand who were recruited through the Psychology Department’s experimental participation program and reimbursed with partial course credit (N = 135, 79%) or recruited through a student employment agency and reimbursed with a small cash payment (N = 36, 21%). Inclusion criteria included being in the young adult age range (18–25 years), having an Internet capable smartphone, identifying as low-FV-consuming (no more than 3 combined servings of FV per day), not being on any anti-depressant medication, and having no known FV allergies. All participants provided written consent to take part by reading and signing their name on a paper consent form (later kept in secured storage). The study and consenting procedures were approved by the University of Otago Human Ethics Committee (#15/010).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Participant Flow Diagram Depicting Reasons for Exclusion and Allocation to Intervention Conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.g001

More detail about the intervention, including the development and complete list of intervention text-messages for the EMI condition, is described in Brookie et al. [26]. Participants attended an initial laboratory session and completed a computerized baseline survey that measured demographics and symptoms of depression and anxiety, among other measures. That evening, participants completed their first of 14 daily diary surveys delivered by hyperlink to their smartphones. The survey measured their daily mood and well-being (see Measures) and was accessible between 7:00 pm and 1:00 am. The next morning, participants attended a clinic in the Human Nutrition department where they were measured for height and weight using standardised techniques [39] and gave a fasting baseline blood sample (4 ml). Immediately after the clinic visit, participants randomly assigned to the EMI condition began to receive their twice-daily intervention messages to increase FV over the next 13 days. Participants in the FVI condition received their bag of FV to consume for the next 13 days. Participants in the control condition were asked to maintain their normal diet. After the 13 intervention days, participants returned to the Human Nutrition clinic and gave a follow up blood sample (4 ml), and completed follow-up measures of depressive symptoms and anxiety.

Measures

Preregistered psychological outcome measures were depressive symptoms, anxiety, negative mood, positive mood, vitality, and flourishing. Curiosity, creativity, and motivation were not registered ahead of time and were therefore considered exploratory.

Demographics and covariates.

Demographic measures were age, gender, and ethnicity. Height and weight measured during the first clinic visit was used to calculate body mass index (BMI) as a possible covariate. Smoking status was tested as a covariate in the vitamin C analysis because smoking increases the metabolism of vitamin C [40]. In the follow-up survey, participants were asked “How often do you currently smoke” by choosing one of five response options: I don’t smoke now, less than once a month, at least once a month, at least once a week, or at least once a day. Participants who smoked at least once a week or day were coded as smokers (1); all others were coded as non-smokers (0).

Depressive symptoms.

Depressive symptoms were measured before and after the intervention using the 20-item Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; [41]). Participants rated their depressive symptomology “in the past week” on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (Rarely or none of the time, < 1 day) to 3 (Most or all of the time, 5–7 days). Responses were summed to give a total depressive symptom score ranging from 0 to 60 (pre α = 0.89; post α = 0.90).

Anxiety.

Anxiety symptoms were measured before and after the intervention using the 7-item Anxiety sub-scale of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; [42]). Participants rated each item on how they “felt recently, including today” on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (Not at all or Only occasionally) to 3 (Very often or Most of the time). Responses were summed to give a total anxiety score ranging from 0 to 21 (baseline α = 0.77; follow up α = 0.78).

Negative and positive mood.

The daily survey included 3-item measures of negative mood (sad, anxious, angry) and positive mood (relaxed, happy, enthusiastic), which captured a range of activation states [43]. Participants rated each adjective on how they “felt today” on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (Extremely). Responses were averaged across the three items each day for a measure of daily negative mood (α = 0.34) and daily positive mood (α = 0.42). The within-person reliabilities for the daily survey data were computed using procedures for nested data recommended by Nezlek [44]. Because of low reliability, we also analysed the items separately in supplementary analyses.

Vitality.

The daily survey included a modified version of the 4-item energy/fatigue scale of the Rand 36-Item Short Form Survey (SF-36 [21, 45]). Participants were asked “today”: Did you feel full of life? Did you have a lot of energy? Did you feel worn out? Did you feel tired? We changed the first item from the original “full of pep” to “full of life” to better capture feelings of vitality and to connect with a young-adult population. Each item was rated on a 6-point scale labelled None of the time, A little of the time, Some of the time, A good bit of the time, Most of the time, and All of the time. Responses were coded from 0 to 100 in 20-point intervals following SF-36 guidelines (reverse-scoring worn out and tired), and summed within each day so that highest total score was 100, indicating higher vitality that day. The modified scale showed adequate within-person reliability (α = 0.70).

Flourishing.

The daily survey included a shortened 3-item version of the 8-item Flourishing Scale that assesses feelings of engagement, purpose in life, and social-emotional connectedness, which previous research shows loads on a single “flourishing” factor [46]. We selected the three highest loading items from a previous dataset and adapted the items for a daily format. The three items were: Today, I was engaged and interested in my daily activities. Today, I led a purposeful and meaningful life. Today, I was a good person and lived a good life. Participants rated each item on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 4 (Neither agree nor disagree) to 7 (Strongly agree). Responses were summed each day for a measure of daily flourishing ranging from 1 to 21 (α = 0.70).

Flourishing behaviors.

The daily survey included three items answered on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (Extremely). The first item measured curiosity: How curious were you today? Did you seek out new things or experiences; look for opportunities to challenge yourself and grow as a person; or embrace the unfamiliar. This question was guided by the content of the Curiosity and Exploration Inventory II [47]. The second item measured creative activity: How creative were you today? Did you come up with novel or original ideas; express yourself in an original and useful way; or spend time doing artistic activities like, music, painting, writing, etc. This question was based on scientific definitions of creativity [48] and has been used in previous research [15]. The third item measured perceived motivation: How motivated were you today? Did you work towards your goals, or feel driven today? This question was designed to capture a broad, conscious sense of motivation including working towards goals [49]. Responses to the three items were averaged each day for an index of daily flourishing behaviors (α = 0.28). Because of low reliability, these items were also analysed separately in supplementary analyses.

Vitamin C and carotenoids.

A 4 ml fasting blood sample was taken before and after the intervention. Samples were kept in dark conditions on ice and centrifuged within 1 hour of collection. Plasma aliquots were stored at -80C until analysis. Plasma vitamin C was assessed by high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) with electrochemical detection, treated with reducing agent to recover total vitamin C. Total plasma carotenoids were assessed spectrophotometrically (for full blood processing details see [26]). Four people were unable to provide a post-intervention blood sample; therefore, analyses with vitamin C and carotenoids reflected a sample size of 167.

Psychological expectancies.

The follow-up survey included questions about participants’ beliefs about the benefits and virtues of eating FV, as well as candy and fried foods as distractor items, and how participants thought eating these foods made them feel over the last two weeks. For analysis, we focused on the three items related to FV consumption: Do you feel that eating fruits and vegetables is virtuous (righteous, good, moral)? Do you feel better about yourself when you eat more fruits and vegetables? Did you feel that eating fruits and vegetables improved your mood during these two weeks? Each question was answered on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (Very slightly/not at all) to 4 (Extremely). We added these expectancy items one third of the way through data collection; therefore, analyses with these items reflected a sample size of 121.

Analyses and results

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics for the demographic and outcome variables. The depression and anxiety scores were within norms for this population [50]. Compliance with the smartphone surveys was high at 90% (12.6/14 surveys completed; range 9 to 14 surveys). There were no differences between the control, EMI, and FVI conditions on survey compliance (F(2,168) = 0.91, p = 0.405), gender (χ2(2, N = 171) = 0.129, p = .938), age (F(2,168) = 1.24, p = 0.291), or BMI (F(2,168) = 1.18, p = 0.309). However, the conditions varied in ethnic composition (χ2(2, N = 171) = 8.31, p = .016) with more non-Europeans in the control condition (51%) than the two intervention conditions (EMI 29%; FVI 28%). Therefore, ethnicity was included as a covariate in the analyses. The analyses consisted of two Analyses of Covariance (ANCOVA) for the measures of depressive symptoms and anxiety, and six growth curve models for the daily measures of positive mood, negative mood, vitality, flourishing, and flourishing behaviors. Based on these seven main analyses, we applied a Bonferroni correction to the alpha level (.05/7 tests) to account for multiple hypothesis testing (p = .007).

thumbnail
Table 1. Sample Characteristics and Descriptive Statistics for the Psychological Outcome Variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.t001

Table 2 presents the raw scores for depressive symptoms and anxiety for participants in each condition. There were no differences at baseline between the three groups (depressive symptoms, F(2,168) = 0.19, p = 0.830; anxiety, F(2,168) = 0.88, p = 0.419). Results of a 3 (x 2) mixed ANCOVA with condition as the between-subjects variable, time as the within-subjects variable, and ethnicity as the covariate showed only a main effect of time in predicting depressive symptoms (F(1, 167) = 5.01, p = .027, Partial η2 = .029), no main effect of condition (F(2, 167) = 0.212, p = .809, Partial η2 = .003) and no time x condition interaction (F(2, 167) = 0.412, p = .663, Partial η2 = .005). Similar patterns were found for anxiety (time, F(1, 167) = 3.49, p = .063, Partial η2 = .020; condition, F(2, 167) = 1.33, p = .268, Partial η2 = .016; time x condition, F(2, 167) = 0.15, p = .859, Partial η2 = .002). The non-significant time x condition coefficients indicated that the interventions did not reduce depressive symptoms or anxiety relative to control.

thumbnail
Table 2. Changes in Depressive Symptoms and Anxiety for the Three Intervention Conditions, Unadjusted for Ethnicity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.t002

The daily survey data were analyzed using the Hierarchical Linear Modeling program to account for dependency among nested data (HLM v.6.08; [51]). We modelled linear growth curves to assess whether participants in the experimental conditions reported greater improvements in mood and well-being over time relative to participants in the control condition. Growth curves were modelled for each participant for each outcome using robust standard errors, as follows: This set of equations determined each person’s negative mood at baseline (B0), how negative mood changed over time for each participant (B1), and how these changes varied by experimental condition (G11; G12). Time was recoded so that 0 was day 1. G11 tested change in the EMI condition against change in the control condition (using Condition Dummy 1; control = 0; EMI = 1; FVI = 0). A significant G11 indicated that participants in the EMI condition exhibited a different pattern of change compared to participants in the control condition. G12 tested change in the FVI condition against the change in the control condition (using Condition Dummy 2; control = 0; EMI = 0; FVI = 1). A significant G12 indicated that participants in the FVI condition exhibited a different pattern of change compared to participants in the control condition. Thus, the null hypothesis was that participants in the EMI / FVI conditions would report similar changes over time as the control participants. For significant group differences, we plotted the growth patterns for the three groups and computed tests of the simple slopes. We also tested an additional set of dummy codes to compare the EMI and FVI groups in subsequent analyses. Ethnicity (0 = European; 1 = non-European) was entered as a covariate in the Level-2 intercept equation. Ethnicity was also entered as a covariate in the Level-2 slope equation but was not significant, did not affect results, and was removed from final models for simplicity.

Table 3 presents the growth curve results. Participants in the two experimental conditions did not show any improvements in negative or positive mood relative to participants in the control condition. Supplementary analyses showed similar null effects when analysing the mood items separately (see S2 Table) with an exception: happy showed significant group differences in change over time. Analysis of simple slopes showed that happiness was stable over time for participants in the FVI condition (B(SE) = -0.001(0.006), p = .845) but happiness decreased for participants in the EMI condition (B(SE) = -0.034(0.010), p = 0.001) and control condition (B(SE) = -0.012(0.008), p = 0.018). The slopes were significantly different only between the FVI and EMI conditions (p = 0.008).

thumbnail
Table 3. Results from Growth Curve Modelling Testing for Differences in Change over Time between the Three Intervention Conditions.

Significant intervention effects are bolded (p < .007).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.t003

However, participants in the FVI condition did show improvements to their psychological well-being compared to participants in the EMI and control conditions. As shown in Table 3, participants given fresh fruit and vegetables to consume (FVI condition) reported significant growth in vitality, flourishing, and flourishing behaviors compared to participants in the EMI and control conditions. Fig 2 presents the growth patterns for these outcomes. As shown in the top of Fig 2, participants in the FVI condition reported lower starting vitality than the other groups, but their rate of improvement was greater compared to both the EMI and control conditions. Analysis of simple slopes showed that participants in the FVI condition reported significant growth in vitality over time (B(SE) = 0.455(0.200), p = .023) whereas participants in EMI and control conditions were unchanged over time (EMI B(SE) = -0.192(0.187), p = 0.307; control B(SE) = -0.336(0.204), p = 0.101). Group differences were even more apparent for the measure of flourishing. As shown in the middle of Fig 2, participants in the FVI condition reported significantly greater increases in flourishing compared to participants in the control and EMI conditions. Analysis of the simple slopes showed that participants in the FVI condition reported significant growth in flourishing over time (B(SE) = 0.010(0.033), p = .004) whereas participants in the EMI and control conditions were unchanged over time (EMI B(SE) = -0.038(0.031), p = 0.215; control B(SE) = -0.031(0.027), p = 0.242). And, shown in the bottom of Fig 2, participants in the FVI condition also reported significantly greater growth in flourishing behaviors compared to participants in the control condition. Simple slope analyses revealed a trend growth in flourishing behaviors for participants in the FVI condition (B(SE) = 0.013(0.007), p = .053), a trend decrease for participants in the EMI condition (B(SE) = -0.009(0.006), p = .093), and a significant decrease for participants in the control condition (B(SE) = -0.012(0.006), p = .038). When we analyzed the three flourishing behaviors separately, all three showed similar patterns, but the strongest effects were found for motivation (see S2). Fig 3 presents the growth patterns for motivation showing that participants in the FVI condition reported significantly increasing motivation over time (B(SE) = 0.019(0.010), p = .045), whereas participants in the EMI and control conditions were unchanged over time (EMI B(SE) = -0.005(0.011), p = 0.652; control B(SE) = -0.011(0.008), p = 0.218).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Changes in vitality, flourishing, and flourishing behaviors (composite of daily curiosity, creativity, and motivation) for the control, ecological momentary intervention (EMI), and fruit and vegetable intervention (FVI) conditions.

Significant simple slopes are indicated. * p < .05; * p < .01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Changes in self-reported motivation for the control, ecological momentary intervention (EMI), and fruit and vegetable intervention (FVI) conditions.

Significant simple slopes are indicated. * p < .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.g003

In terms of effect sizes, being assigned to the FVI condition (vs. control) predicted 8.7% of the variance in growth in vitality, 10.5% of the growth in flourishing, and 18.4% of the growth in flourishing behaviors using variance-explained effect size estimates of the time slopes [52].

Additional analyses testing for non-linear change over time using time and time squared as level-1 predictors found no evidence for curvilinear changes over time or moderation of curvilinear changes by intervention group. Thus, changes in well-being as a function of the intervention were strictly linear.

Lastly, none of these changes in well-being were mediated by post-test vitamin C, post-test carotenoid levels, or psychological expectancies. Following procedures for testing mediation with a multicategorical independent variable [53], we added each mediator as a separate Level-2 predictor (grand-mean centered) plus the two condition dummy codes (ConditionD1 and ConditionD2) to predict both the intercepts (B0s) and time slopes (B1s) for vitality, flourishing, and flourishing behaviors (with ethnicity as a covariate in the intercept equation, as before). Five mediators were tested separately: post-test vitamin C levels, post-test carotenoid levels, expectancy 1 (eating FV is virtuous), expectancy 2 (I feel better about myself when eating more FV), and expectancy 3 (eating FV improved my mood). None of the condition coefficients G11 or G12 changed when entering vitamin C, carotenoids, or the three psychological expectancy questions into the model. The fact that the total effects (c) were nearly equivalent to the direct effects (c’) when partialling out each indirect mediational pathway (ab) argues against mediation by these variables.

Discussion

Giving young adults fruits and vegetables (FV) to eat, but not reminding them to eat their FV, improved several aspects of psychological well-being over a two-week period. Despite both intervention groups reporting modestly higher and similar consumption of FV relative to control (3.7 vs. 2.8 daily servings) [26], only young adults who were given two weeks’ worth of FV showed improvements in their feelings of vitality, flourishing, and motivation. The short duration of our study indicated that FV intake translated into improved well-being quite rapidly.

The intervention effects were prominent across measures of well-being but not ill-being such as depression, anxiety, or negative mood. This is an interesting pattern that supports the observational and prospective research linking FV to greater well-being [10, 14, 15]. It is also possible that this pattern might reflect differences in the timescale of effects. The majority of research linking depression to dietary patterns has been longitudinal, meaning that possible differences in ill-being may be established over a much longer period of time rather than our brief two-week period. The accumulation of factors such as low vitality, reduced motivation, and poorer socio-emotional flourishing may precipitate the development of psychological ill-being over time [54]. In saying this, Smith and Rogers [24] did find a difference in self–reported anxiety using the same measure (HADS) over a 10-day period with snacking on one piece of fruit each day compared to snacking on chocolate. However, their participants had higher average baseline anxiety (approximately +1.5 points). Intervention studies with positive outcomes tend to have participants with higher baselines of illness [55], which may explain the discrepancy in our findings.

The lack of psychological improvement among the EMI participants is challenging to interpret. Both the EMI and FVI groups reported higher and equivalent consumption of FV compared to control, but the EMI group did not show any corresponding psychological benefit. Both intervention conditions were given roughly the same amount of monetary goods at the start of the study–a $10 voucher for the EMI condition versus a little more than $10 worth of fruit and vegetables for the FVI condition. One possibility is that this difference might be due to lower control over the type, quality, and preparation of fruit and vegetables eaten by the EMI group. The EMI group were free to choose whatever fruit and vegetables they liked, and when surveyed, we found that they were more likely to eat cooked vegetables in casseroles or mixed into their main meals [26]. By contrast, for the FVI group, we chose high quality produce, which was mostly eaten raw (including the carrots, eaten as snacks). Some researchers have shown that more optimal psychological outcomes are associated with the consumption of fresh fruit and raw vegetables/ salads, but not cooked vegetables [56, 57]. It is also possible that regular text-reminders (twice daily for two weeks) might have annoyed participants in the EMI condition and wiped out any gains in well-being. However, our experience is that participant burden is relatively low with receiving only two text messages per day. Furthermore, informal feedback from the EMI participants was mostly positive; participants reported that the messages were not particularly intrusive and were effective at making them more aware about what they ate [26]. Alternatively, it could be that giving FV to participants triggered more intrinsic rather than extrinsic regulation of behavior. A previous study found that households given boxes of free FV were reluctant to waste the food and therefore had to overcome internal conflicts in their personal value systems and find solutions they could live with–contributing to a largely internal source of motivation to consume the FV they had been given [58]. Given that intrinsic goals tend to be more rewarding and lead to greater well-being [59], this could potentially explain the psychological improvement in the FVI condition, but not in the EMI condition.

The significant changes in well-being occurred despite relatively small changes in FV consumption (+ 1 serving per day more than control, and +1.2 servings from baseline for the FVI condition). In fact, participants in the FVI condition still fell short of the “5+ a day” FV servings target [27], let alone the optimal 7–9 daily servings that has been associated with long-term health [1]. Psychological improvements could have been larger had participants in the experimental conditions eaten even more FV (or higher quality FV) or had they achieved saturation levels of vitamin C and carotenoids. As we reported in Brookie et al. [26], the micronutrient increases in vitamin C and carotenoids were modest for our intervention conditions and well-below saturation levels, suggesting that there was still room for improvement. In future research, it would be important to increase consumption more than we did here.

Neither vitamin C and carotenoids nor psychological expectancies about FV mediated the psychological benefits of FV consumption in this study. This was despite us choosing the two most responsive micronutrient biomarkers of FV consumption [32] and measuring a range of psychological expectancies. The literature on micronutrient status and mental health suggests that single nutrients are unlikely to play a large role in the protection against mental illness [60]. Instead, better mental health may arise from the cumulative effects of a broad spectrum of vitamins, minerals, and antioxidants [61]. Future research should consider how FV consumption affects well-being through multiple pathways, including through improved blood flow to the brain [62] or changes in gut microbiota [63, 64].

Strengths of the study included the three-arm randomized design that compared two experimental conditions to a diet-as-usual control condition; use of a smartphone survey to track mood and well-being in near-to-real time which provided a more accurate assessment than retrospective self-report; high compliance with the smartphone survey; and, inclusion of blood samples as an objective marker of FV consumption. Limitations included issues with measurement reliability for some of the diary measures, which was a consequence of efforts to keep the survey short. This could explain why we did not see intervention effects on mood. Future research should include a more extensive measure of mood, such as the 9-item measure of positive mood previously found associated with FV [11, 15]. Another limitation is the short timeframe of our intervention (two weeks) and lack of longer-term follow-up. In this regard, our approach was more of a ‘light touch’ intervention to evoke near-term behavioral change. For this reason, we consider these results preliminary, offering proof of concept that small changes to FV consumption over a short time period may confer changes in well-being.

Our findings have implications for campaigns designed to increase FV consumption. FV campaigns reflect an ‘information is power’ ethos and largely consist of educative programmes operating through child health and wellness services, schools, communities, and public service television ads. However, our research suggests that simply educating people about FV and reminding them to eat their recommended daily intake may not be sufficient in ensuring the wider population reaps the psychological benefits of FV consumption. It is already established that successful interventions tend to be more personal and more intensive [55], but perhaps greater emphasis needs to be placed on actually providing people with fresh FV (stocking more FV in dorms, cafeterias, workplaces, substituting fruit for dessert, and offering free fruit for people when they shop). Additionally, conveying the immediate psychological benefits of FV consumption may have more impact on behavior. Behavioral change messages are more effective when they are immediately relevant to the target population [65] suggesting that near-term benefits–such as increases in vitality and motivation–may be more salient than longer-term health benefits–such as the prevention of heart disease. This may be particularly pertinent to young people, who are endowed with feelings of immortality and quite focused on the ‘here and now’.

Conclusions

Providing young adults with high-quality FV, not texting them reminders to eat more FV and giving them a voucher, resulted in improvements to their psychological well-being over a two-week period. This is the first study to show that providing high-quality FV to young adults can result in short-term improvements in vitality, flourishing, and motivation. Findings provide initial validation of a causal relationship between FV and well-being, suggesting that large-scale intervention studies are warranted.

Supporting information

S1 File. Pre- and Post-intervention Variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.s001

(SAV)

S1 Table. CONSORT Checklist for Reporting a Randomized Trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.s003

(PDF)

S2 Table. Separate Analyses of Mood Items and Flourishing Behaviours.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206.s004

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Hadyn Youens for web programming and members of the Daily Experiences Lab for their assistance with data collection and running of the blood clinic. We thank Angel Temple and Ashley Duncan for their work taking and preparing bloods and Simone Bayer for technical assistance with the analysis of biomarkers.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: TC KB.
  2. Data curation: TC KB.
  3. Formal analysis: TC KB MV AC.
  4. Funding acquisition: TC.
  5. Investigation: KB.
  6. Methodology: TC KB.
  7. Project administration: KB.
  8. Resources: KB.
  9. Supervision: TC.
  10. Visualization: TC KB.
  11. Writing – original draft: TC KB.
  12. Writing – review & editing: TC KB MV AC LM.

References

  1. 1. Oyebode O, Gordon-Dseagu V, Walker A, Mindell JS. Fruit and vegetable consumption and all-cause, cancer and CVD mortality: analysis of Health Survey for England data. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2014 Mar 31:jech-2013.
  2. 2. Boffetta P, Couto E, Wichmann J, Ferrari P, Trichopoulos D, Bueno-de-Mesquita HB, et al. Fruit and vegetable intake and overall cancer risk in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC). Journal of the National Cancer Institute. 2010 Apr 21;102(8):529–37. pmid:20371762
  3. 3. Bellavia A, Larsson SC, Bottai M, Wolk A, Orsini N. Fruit and vegetable consumption and all-cause mortality: a dose-response analysis. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2013 Aug 1;98(2):454–9. pmid:23803880
  4. 4. Adams TB, Colner W. The association of multiple risk factors with fruit and vegetable intake among a nationwide sample of college students. Journal of American College Health. 2008 Jan 1;56(4):455–61. pmid:18316291
  5. 5. Allgöwer A, Wardle J, Steptoe A. Depressive symptoms, social support, and personal health behaviors in young men and women. Health Psychology. 2001 May;20(3):223. pmid:11403220
  6. 6. Carr AC, Bozonet SM, Pullar JM, Vissers MC. Mood improvement in young adult males following supplementation with gold kiwifruit, a high-vitamin C food. Journal of Nutritional Science. 2013;2:e24. pmid:25191573
  7. 7. Jacka FN, Kremer PJ, Leslie ER, Berk M, Patton GC, Toumbourou JW, et al. Associations between diet quality and depressed mood in adolescents: results from the Australian Healthy Neighbourhoods Study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2010 Jan 1;44(5):435–42. pmid:20397785
  8. 8. Kulkarni AA, Swinburn BA, Utter J. Associations between diet quality and mental health in socially disadvantaged New Zealand adolescents. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2015 Jan 1;69(1):79–83. pmid:25028085
  9. 9. McMartin SE, Jacka FN, Colman I. The association between fruit and vegetable consumption and mental health disorders: evidence from five waves of a national survey of Canadians. Preventive Medicine. 2013 Apr 30;56(3):225–30.
  10. 10. Blanchflower DG, Oswald AJ, Stewart-Brown S. Is psychological well-being linked to the consumption of fruit and vegetables?. Social Indicators Research. 2013 Dec 1;114(3):785–801.
  11. 11. White BA, Horwath CC, Conner TS. Many apples a day keep the blues away–Daily experiences of negative and positive affect and food consumption in young adults. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2013 Nov 1;18(4):782–98. pmid:23347122
  12. 12. Grant N, Wardle J, Steptoe A. The relationship between life satisfaction and health behavior: a cross-cultural analysis of young adults. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009 Sep 1;16(3):259–68. pmid:19319695
  13. 13. Lengyel CO, Tate RB, Obirek Blatz AK. The relationships between food group consumption, self-rated health, and life satisfaction of community-dwelling canadian older men: the manitoba follow-up study. Journal of Nutrition for the Elderly. 2009 May 29;28(2):158–73. pmid:21184363
  14. 14. Mujcic R, Oswald A J.. Evolution of well-being and happiness after increases in consumption of fruit and vegetables. American Journal of Public Health. 2016 Aug;106(8):1504–10. pmid:27400354
  15. 15. Conner TS, Brookie KL, Richardson AC, Polak MA. On carrots and curiosity: eating fruit and vegetables is associated with greater flourishing in daily life. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2015 May 1;20(2):413–27. pmid:25080035
  16. 16. Akbaraly TN, Sabia S, Shipley MJ, Batty GD, Kivimaki M. Adherence to healthy dietary guidelines and future depressive symptoms: evidence for sex differentials in the Whitehall II study. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2013 Feb 1;97(2):419–27. pmid:23283506
  17. 17. Jacka FN, Kremer PJ, Berk M, de Silva-Sanigorski AM, Moodie M, Leslie ER, et al. A prospective study of diet quality and mental health in adolescents. PLoS One. 2011 Sep 21;6(9):e24805. pmid:21957462
  18. 18. Le Port A, Gueguen A, Kesse-Guyot E, Melchior M, Lemogne C, Nabi H, et al. Association between dietary patterns and depressive symptoms over time: a 10-year follow-up study of the GAZEL cohort. PLoS One. 2012 Dec 12;7(12):e51593. pmid:23251585
  19. 19. Rienks J, Dobson AJ, Mishra GD. Mediterranean dietary pattern and prevalence and incidence of depressive symptoms in mid-aged women: results from a large community-based prospective study. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2013 Jan 1;67(1):75–82. pmid:23212131
  20. 20. Skarupski KA, Tangney CC, Li H, Evans DA, Morris MC. Mediterranean diet and depressive symptoms among older adults over time. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging. 2013 May 1;17(5):441–5.
  21. 21. Ware JE Jr, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Medical Care. 1992 Jun 1:473–83.
  22. 22. Steptoe A, Perkins-Porras L, Hilton S, Rink E, Cappuccio FP. Quality of life and self-rated health in relation to changes in fruit and vegetable intake and in plasma vitamins C and E in a randomised trial of behavioural and nutritional education counselling. British Journal of Nutrition. 2004 Jul 1;92(01):177–84.
  23. 23. McNair DM, Heuchert JW. P.(2005). Profile of mood states. Technical update. North Tonawanda: Multi-Health Systems.
  24. 24. Smith AP, Rogers R. Positive effects of a healthy snack (fruit) versus an unhealthy snack (chocolate/crisps) on subjective reports of mental and physical health: A preliminary intervention study. Frontiers in Nutrition. 2014;1.
  25. 25. Boehm JK, Williams DR, Rimm EB, Ryff C, Kubzansky LD. The Association between optimism and serum antioxidants in the midlife in the United States study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2013 Jan;75(1):2. pmid:23257932
  26. 26. Brookie KL, Mainvil LA, Carr AC, Vissers MC, Conner TS. The development and effectiveness of an ecological momentary intervention to increase daily fruit and vegetable consumption in low-consuming young adults. Appetite. 2017 Jan 1;108:32–41. pmid:27642037
  27. 27. Ministry of Health. Eating and Activity Guidelines for New Zealand Adults. Wellington: Ministry of Health. 2015.
  28. 28. Krebs-Smith SM, Guenther PM, Subar AF, Kirkpatrick SI, Dodd KW. Americans do not meet federal dietary recommendations. The Journal of nutrition. 2010 Oct 1;140(10):1832–8. pmid:20702750
  29. 29. Smith C, Gray AR, Mainvil LA, Fleming EA, Parnell WR. Secular changes in intakes of foods among New Zealand adults from 1997 to 2008/09. Public Health Nutrition. 2015 Dec 1;18(18):3249–59. pmid:25857451
  30. 30. University of Otago and Ministry of Health. A Focus on Nutrition: Key findings of the 2008/09 New Zealand Adult Nutrition Survey. Wellington, NZ: Ministry of Health. 2011.
  31. 31. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist. 2000 May;55(5):469. pmid:10842426
  32. 32. Baldrick FR, Woodside JV, Elborn JS, Young IS, McKinley MC. Biomarkers of fruit and vegetable intake in human intervention studies: a systematic review. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition. 2011 Oct 1;51(9):795–815. pmid:21888531
  33. 33. Conner T Barrett L. Trends in Ambulatory Self-Report. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2012;74(4):327–337. pmid:22582330
  34. 34. World Health Organization. Constitution of the World Health Organization. Geneva. 1948.
  35. 35. Tsai AC, Chang TL, Chi SH. Frequent consumption of vegetables predicts lower risk of depression in older Taiwanese–results of a prospective population-based study. Public Health Nutrition. 2012 Jun 1;15(06):1087–92.
  36. 36. Ryan RM, Deci EL. From ego depletion to vitality: Theory and findings concerning the facilitation of energy available to the self. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2008 Mar 1;2(2):702–17.
  37. 37. May JM, Qu ZC, Meredith ME. Mechanisms of ascorbic acid stimulation of norepinephrine synthesis in neuronal cells. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications. 2012 Sep 14;426(1):148–52. pmid:22925890
  38. 38. Rooney C, McKinley MC, Woodside JV. The potential role of fruit and vegetables in aspects of psychological well-being: a review of the literature and future directions. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 2013 Nov 1;72(04):420–32.
  39. 39. Lohman TJ, Roache AF, Martorell R. Anthropometric standardization reference manual. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 1992 Aug 1;24(8):952.
  40. 40. Wei W, Kim Y, Boudreau N. Association of smoking with serum and dietary levels of antioxidants in adults: NHANES III, 1988–1994. American Journal of Public Health. 2001 Feb;91(2):258. pmid:11211635
  41. 41. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977 Jun 1;1(3):385–401.
  42. 42. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1983 Jun 1;67(6):361–70. pmid:6880820
  43. 43. Barrett LF, Russell JA. The structure of current affect controversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1999 Feb 1;8(1):10–4.
  44. 44. Nezlek JB. Multilevel modeling analyses of diary-style data. In Mehl MR, Conner TS, editors. Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. Guilford Press. New York: NY. 2012. p. 257–383.
  45. 45. Hays RD, Sherbourne CD, Mazel RM. The rand 36‐item health survey 1.0. Health Economics. 1993 Oct 1;2(3):217–27. pmid:8275167
  46. 46. Diener E, Wirtz D, Tov W, Kim-Prieto C, Choi DW, Oishi S, et al. New well-being measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators Research. 2010 Jun 1;97(2):143–56.
  47. 47. Kashdan TB, Gallagher MW, Silvia PJ, Winterstein BP, Breen WE, Terhar D, et al. The curiosity and exploration inventory-II: Development, factor structure, and psychometrics. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009 Dec 31;43(6):987–98. pmid:20160913
  48. 48. Hennessey BA, Amabile TM. Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology,2010, 61(1). p. 569–598.
  49. 49. Touré‐Tillery M, Fishbach A. How to measure motivation: A guide for the experimental social psychologist. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2014 Jul 1;8(7):328–41.
  50. 50. Dodd LJ, Al-Nakeeb Y, Nevill A, Forshaw MJ. Lifestyle risk factors of students: a cluster analytical approach. Preventive Medicine. 2010 Jul 31;51(1):73–7. pmid:20385163
  51. 51. Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS, Congdon RT. HLM 6.08. Hierarchical Linear and Nonlinear Modeling. Lincolnwood: Scientific Software International. 2009.
  52. 52. Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods (Vol. 1). Sage. Thousand Oaks: California. 2002.
  53. 53. Hayes AF, Preacher KJ. Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 2014 Nov 1;67(3):451–70. pmid:24188158
  54. 54. Keyes CL. Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: a complementary strategy for improving national mental health. American Psychologist. 2007 Feb;62(2):95. pmid:17324035
  55. 55. Pomerleau J, Lock K, Knai C, McKee M. Interventions designed to increase adult fruit and vegetable intake can be effective: a systematic review of the literature. The Journal of Nutrition. 2005 Oct 1;135(10):2486–95. pmid:16177217
  56. 56. Appleton KM, Woodside JV, Yarnell JW, Arveiler D, Haas B, Amouyel P, et al. Depressed mood and dietary fish intake: direct relationship or indirect relationship as a result of diet and lifestyle?. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2007 Dec 31;104(1):217–23.
  57. 57. El Ansari W, Suominen S, Berg-Beckhoff G. Mood and food at the University of Turku in Finland: nutritional correlates of perceived stress are most pronounced among overweight students. International Journal of Public Health. 2015 Sep 1;60(6):707–16. pmid:26202822
  58. 58. Carty SA, Mainvil LA, Coveney JD. Exploring family home food environments: Household resources needed to utilise weekly deliveries of free fruits and vegetables. Nutrition and Dietetics. 2016 Jul 1.
  59. 59. Kasser T, Ryan MR. Be careful what you wish for: Optimal functioning and the relative attainment of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. In Schmuck P, Sheldon MK, editors. Life goals and wellbeing: Towards a positive psychology of human striving. Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers, 2001. p. 116–131.
  60. 60. Rucklidge JJ, Johnstone J, Kaplan BJ. Magic bullet thinking-why do we continue to perpetuate this fallacy?. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2013 Aug 1;203(2):154-.
  61. 61. Rucklidge JJ, Kaplan BJ. Broad-spectrum micronutrient formulas for the treatment of psychiatric symptoms: a systematic review. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics. 2013 Jan 1;13(1):49–73. pmid:23253391
  62. 62. Ghosh D, Scheepens A. Vascular action of polyphenols. Molecular Nutrition & Food Research. 2009 Mar 1;53(3):322–31.
  63. 63. Cryan JF, Dinan TG. Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012 Oct 1;13(10):701–12. pmid:22968153
  64. 64. Dash S, Clarke G, Berk M, Jacka FN. The gut microbiome and diet in psychiatry: focus on depression. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2015 Jan 1;28(1):1–6. pmid:25415497
  65. 65. Epton T, Norman P, Harris P, Webb T, Snowsill FA, Sheeran P. Development of theory-based health messages: three-phase programme of formative research. Health Promotion International. 2014 Feb 5:dau005.